The prospects for an evolutionary psychology: Human language and human reasoning View Full Text


Ontology type: schema:ScholarlyArticle     


Article Info

DATE

1996-11

AUTHORS

Robert C. Richardson

ABSTRACT

Evolutionary psychology purports to explain human capacities as adaptations to an ancestral environment. A complete explanation of human language or human reasoning as adaptations depends on assessing an historical claim, that these capacities evolved under the pressure of natural selection and are prevalent because they provided systematic advantages to our ancestors. An outline of the character of the information needed in order to offer complete adaptation explanations is drawn from Robert Brandon (1990), and explanations offered for the evolution of language and reasoning within evolutionary psychology are evaluated. Pinker and Bloom's (1992) defense of human language as an adaptation for verbal communication, Robert Nozick's (1993) account of the evolutionary origin of rationality, and Cosmides and Tooby's (1992) explanation of human reasoning as an adaptation for social exchange, are discussed in light of what is known, and what is not known, about the history of human evolution. In each case, though a plausible case is made that these capacities are adaptations, there is not enough known to offer even a semblance of an explanation of the origin of these capacities. These explanations of the origin of human thought and language are simply speculations lacking the kind of detailed historical information required for an evolutionary explanation of an adaptation. More... »

PAGES

541-557

Journal

TITLE

Minds and Machines

ISSUE

4

VOLUME

6

Author Affiliations

Identifiers

URI

http://scigraph.springernature.com/pub.10.1007/bf00389658

DOI

http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00389658

DIMENSIONS

https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1086196911


Indexing Status Check whether this publication has been indexed by Scopus and Web Of Science using the SN Indexing Status Tool
Incoming Citations Browse incoming citations for this publication using opencitations.net

JSON-LD is the canonical representation for SciGraph data.

TIP: You can open this SciGraph record using an external JSON-LD service: JSON-LD Playground Google SDTT

[
  {
    "@context": "https://springernature.github.io/scigraph/jsonld/sgcontext.json", 
    "about": [
      {
        "id": "http://purl.org/au-research/vocabulary/anzsrc-for/2008/0604", 
        "inDefinedTermSet": "http://purl.org/au-research/vocabulary/anzsrc-for/2008/", 
        "name": "Genetics", 
        "type": "DefinedTerm"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "http://purl.org/au-research/vocabulary/anzsrc-for/2008/06", 
        "inDefinedTermSet": "http://purl.org/au-research/vocabulary/anzsrc-for/2008/", 
        "name": "Biological Sciences", 
        "type": "DefinedTerm"
      }
    ], 
    "author": [
      {
        "affiliation": {
          "alternateName": "University of Cincinnati", 
          "id": "https://www.grid.ac/institutes/grid.24827.3b", 
          "name": [
            "Department of Philosophy, University of Cincinnati, 45221-0371, Cincinnati, OH, USA"
          ], 
          "type": "Organization"
        }, 
        "familyName": "Richardson", 
        "givenName": "Robert C.", 
        "type": "Person"
      }
    ], 
    "citation": [
      {
        "id": "sg:pub.10.1007/bf00851985", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1008221242", 
          "https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00851985"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "sg:pub.10.1007/bf00851985", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1008221242", 
          "https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00851985"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2504(08)60202-0", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1015160232"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "sg:pub.10.1038/hdy.1970.36", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1020650372", 
          "https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1970.36"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "sg:pub.10.1038/hdy.1970.36", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1020650372", 
          "https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1970.36"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "sg:pub.10.1038/hdy.1966.42", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1026526436", 
          "https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1966.42"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "sg:pub.10.1038/hdy.1966.42", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1026526436", 
          "https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1966.42"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://doi.org/10.1016/0039-3681(78)90005-5", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1032538227"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://doi.org/10.1086/282258", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1058592381"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://doi.org/10.1101/sqb.1957.022.01.037", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1060403273"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1077358931", 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1080620549", 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02133.x", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1085730784"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02134.x", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1085731541"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1994.tb02208.x", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1085733943"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }, 
      {
        "id": "https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02132.x", 
        "sameAs": [
          "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1085736353"
        ], 
        "type": "CreativeWork"
      }
    ], 
    "datePublished": "1996-11", 
    "datePublishedReg": "1996-11-01", 
    "description": "Evolutionary psychology purports to explain human capacities as adaptations to an ancestral environment. A complete explanation of human language or human reasoning as adaptations depends on assessing an historical claim, that these capacities evolved under the pressure of natural selection and are prevalent because they provided systematic advantages to our ancestors. An outline of the character of the information needed in order to offer complete adaptation explanations is drawn from Robert Brandon (1990), and explanations offered for the evolution of language and reasoning within evolutionary psychology are evaluated. Pinker and Bloom's (1992) defense of human language as an adaptation for verbal communication, Robert Nozick's (1993) account of the evolutionary origin of rationality, and Cosmides and Tooby's (1992) explanation of human reasoning as an adaptation for social exchange, are discussed in light of what is known, and what is not known, about the history of human evolution. In each case, though a plausible case is made that these capacities are adaptations, there is not enough known to offer even a semblance of an explanation of the origin of these capacities. These explanations of the origin of human thought and language are simply speculations lacking the kind of detailed historical information required for an evolutionary explanation of an adaptation.", 
    "genre": "research_article", 
    "id": "sg:pub.10.1007/bf00389658", 
    "inLanguage": [
      "en"
    ], 
    "isAccessibleForFree": false, 
    "isPartOf": [
      {
        "id": "sg:journal.1052496", 
        "issn": [
          "0924-6495", 
          "1572-8641"
        ], 
        "name": "Minds and Machines", 
        "type": "Periodical"
      }, 
      {
        "issueNumber": "4", 
        "type": "PublicationIssue"
      }, 
      {
        "type": "PublicationVolume", 
        "volumeNumber": "6"
      }
    ], 
    "name": "The prospects for an evolutionary psychology: Human language and human reasoning", 
    "pagination": "541-557", 
    "productId": [
      {
        "name": "readcube_id", 
        "type": "PropertyValue", 
        "value": [
          "658dcab7b45bb3e4c71080f779710754ea03631c22385a963866bceb22519c49"
        ]
      }, 
      {
        "name": "doi", 
        "type": "PropertyValue", 
        "value": [
          "10.1007/bf00389658"
        ]
      }, 
      {
        "name": "dimensions_id", 
        "type": "PropertyValue", 
        "value": [
          "pub.1086196911"
        ]
      }
    ], 
    "sameAs": [
      "https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00389658", 
      "https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1086196911"
    ], 
    "sdDataset": "articles", 
    "sdDatePublished": "2019-04-11T01:04", 
    "sdLicense": "https://scigraph.springernature.com/explorer/license/", 
    "sdPublisher": {
      "name": "Springer Nature - SN SciGraph project", 
      "type": "Organization"
    }, 
    "sdSource": "s3://com-uberresearch-data-dimensions-target-20181106-alternative/cleanup/v134/2549eaecd7973599484d7c17b260dba0a4ecb94b/merge/v9/a6c9fde33151104705d4d7ff012ea9563521a3ce/jats-lookup/v90/0000000001_0000000264/records_8697_00000497.jsonl", 
    "type": "ScholarlyArticle", 
    "url": "http://link.springer.com/10.1007/BF00389658"
  }
]
 

Download the RDF metadata as:  json-ld nt turtle xml License info

HOW TO GET THIS DATA PROGRAMMATICALLY:

JSON-LD is a popular format for linked data which is fully compatible with JSON.

curl -H 'Accept: application/ld+json' 'https://scigraph.springernature.com/pub.10.1007/bf00389658'

N-Triples is a line-based linked data format ideal for batch operations.

curl -H 'Accept: application/n-triples' 'https://scigraph.springernature.com/pub.10.1007/bf00389658'

Turtle is a human-readable linked data format.

curl -H 'Accept: text/turtle' 'https://scigraph.springernature.com/pub.10.1007/bf00389658'

RDF/XML is a standard XML format for linked data.

curl -H 'Accept: application/rdf+xml' 'https://scigraph.springernature.com/pub.10.1007/bf00389658'


 

This table displays all metadata directly associated to this object as RDF triples.

100 TRIPLES      21 PREDICATES      40 URIs      19 LITERALS      7 BLANK NODES

Subject Predicate Object
1 sg:pub.10.1007/bf00389658 schema:about anzsrc-for:06
2 anzsrc-for:0604
3 schema:author N7602793ebc5d470298db3db711171c15
4 schema:citation sg:pub.10.1007/bf00851985
5 sg:pub.10.1038/hdy.1966.42
6 sg:pub.10.1038/hdy.1970.36
7 https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1077358931
8 https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1080620549
9 https://doi.org/10.1016/0039-3681(78)90005-5
10 https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2504(08)60202-0
11 https://doi.org/10.1086/282258
12 https://doi.org/10.1101/sqb.1957.022.01.037
13 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02132.x
14 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02133.x
15 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02134.x
16 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1994.tb02208.x
17 schema:datePublished 1996-11
18 schema:datePublishedReg 1996-11-01
19 schema:description Evolutionary psychology purports to explain human capacities as adaptations to an ancestral environment. A complete explanation of human language or human reasoning as adaptations depends on assessing an historical claim, that these capacities evolved under the pressure of natural selection and are prevalent because they provided systematic advantages to our ancestors. An outline of the character of the information needed in order to offer complete adaptation explanations is drawn from Robert Brandon (1990), and explanations offered for the evolution of language and reasoning within evolutionary psychology are evaluated. Pinker and Bloom's (1992) defense of human language as an adaptation for verbal communication, Robert Nozick's (1993) account of the evolutionary origin of rationality, and Cosmides and Tooby's (1992) explanation of human reasoning as an adaptation for social exchange, are discussed in light of what is known, and what is not known, about the history of human evolution. In each case, though a plausible case is made that these capacities are adaptations, there is not enough known to offer even a semblance of an explanation of the origin of these capacities. These explanations of the origin of human thought and language are simply speculations lacking the kind of detailed historical information required for an evolutionary explanation of an adaptation.
20 schema:genre research_article
21 schema:inLanguage en
22 schema:isAccessibleForFree false
23 schema:isPartOf Nc20b974d57d74a5497fb0ab09c436a35
24 Nde3551db9c414c3b91206243a4243c8e
25 sg:journal.1052496
26 schema:name The prospects for an evolutionary psychology: Human language and human reasoning
27 schema:pagination 541-557
28 schema:productId N44efc9f1283a4f48ab20ce32c89cca17
29 N5870a1ecc0944f0d864f4f7c49afcdae
30 N6900b58e668a45be9c63fb5077e01d72
31 schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1086196911
32 https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00389658
33 schema:sdDatePublished 2019-04-11T01:04
34 schema:sdLicense https://scigraph.springernature.com/explorer/license/
35 schema:sdPublisher Ne9b8a94382c34b78b6a9cea19ac74366
36 schema:url http://link.springer.com/10.1007/BF00389658
37 sgo:license sg:explorer/license/
38 sgo:sdDataset articles
39 rdf:type schema:ScholarlyArticle
40 N44efc9f1283a4f48ab20ce32c89cca17 schema:name readcube_id
41 schema:value 658dcab7b45bb3e4c71080f779710754ea03631c22385a963866bceb22519c49
42 rdf:type schema:PropertyValue
43 N5870a1ecc0944f0d864f4f7c49afcdae schema:name dimensions_id
44 schema:value pub.1086196911
45 rdf:type schema:PropertyValue
46 N6900b58e668a45be9c63fb5077e01d72 schema:name doi
47 schema:value 10.1007/bf00389658
48 rdf:type schema:PropertyValue
49 N7602793ebc5d470298db3db711171c15 rdf:first Nd3ee509f7439415294120c7af9b4e9e0
50 rdf:rest rdf:nil
51 Nc20b974d57d74a5497fb0ab09c436a35 schema:volumeNumber 6
52 rdf:type schema:PublicationVolume
53 Nd3ee509f7439415294120c7af9b4e9e0 schema:affiliation https://www.grid.ac/institutes/grid.24827.3b
54 schema:familyName Richardson
55 schema:givenName Robert C.
56 rdf:type schema:Person
57 Nde3551db9c414c3b91206243a4243c8e schema:issueNumber 4
58 rdf:type schema:PublicationIssue
59 Ne9b8a94382c34b78b6a9cea19ac74366 schema:name Springer Nature - SN SciGraph project
60 rdf:type schema:Organization
61 anzsrc-for:06 schema:inDefinedTermSet anzsrc-for:
62 schema:name Biological Sciences
63 rdf:type schema:DefinedTerm
64 anzsrc-for:0604 schema:inDefinedTermSet anzsrc-for:
65 schema:name Genetics
66 rdf:type schema:DefinedTerm
67 sg:journal.1052496 schema:issn 0924-6495
68 1572-8641
69 schema:name Minds and Machines
70 rdf:type schema:Periodical
71 sg:pub.10.1007/bf00851985 schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1008221242
72 https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00851985
73 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
74 sg:pub.10.1038/hdy.1966.42 schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1026526436
75 https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1966.42
76 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
77 sg:pub.10.1038/hdy.1970.36 schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1020650372
78 https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1970.36
79 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
80 https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1077358931 schema:CreativeWork
81 https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1080620549 schema:CreativeWork
82 https://doi.org/10.1016/0039-3681(78)90005-5 schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1032538227
83 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
84 https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2504(08)60202-0 schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1015160232
85 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
86 https://doi.org/10.1086/282258 schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1058592381
87 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
88 https://doi.org/10.1101/sqb.1957.022.01.037 schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1060403273
89 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
90 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02132.x schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1085736353
91 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
92 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02133.x schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1085730784
93 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
94 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb02134.x schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1085731541
95 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
96 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1994.tb02208.x schema:sameAs https://app.dimensions.ai/details/publication/pub.1085733943
97 rdf:type schema:CreativeWork
98 https://www.grid.ac/institutes/grid.24827.3b schema:alternateName University of Cincinnati
99 schema:name Department of Philosophy, University of Cincinnati, 45221-0371, Cincinnati, OH, USA
100 rdf:type schema:Organization
 




Preview window. Press ESC to close (or click here)


...